The Global-personal Act

Authors

  • José Víctor Orón Semper Universidad de Navarra

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.31207/colloquia.v4i0.46

Keywords:

cognition, development, adolescense, global-personal act

Abstract

Since Piaget proposed formal operations as the cognitive act that starts at the end of childhood and matures in adolescence, the cognitive act in adolescents has gradually broken away from the complexity involved in knowing and acting during adolescence. Thus, cognition remains as a technical act and this entails a reduction of what knowing really means during adolescence. We intend to propose an alternative term to such of formal operations, ‘global-personal act’, an act which best represents the actions of adolescents and, therefore, leads to an understanding of adolescent cognition in its true highly existentialist and personal context. The global-personal act certainly is a cognitive act but it is more than that, due to the fact that it places adolescent cognition within the vital complexity experienced by teenagers. Using this term instead of formal operations guarantees not separating knowing from acting and, further, it allows understanding adolescent vital context. In this article, in order to characterize the global-personal act, we study adolescent cognitive act through what neuroscience refers to as simulation. By studying simulation, we will see that it is not possible to break cognition away from the complexity of living and from the whole set of human dimensions. Afterwards, we discover the personal character of adolescent acting, which was learned in childhood. All this allows us to propose a formulation of global-personal act as the act that manifests the fullness of authorship, since it implies a deliberate action and personal adhesion to the action, as it is desired in itself. The consequence of this is unrestricted growth. The term ‘personal’ makes special reference to childhood, and the term ‘global’ refers to adolescence, but without being exclusive to each period.

Neuroscience discoveries will dialogue with Philosophy and Psychology. These three disciplines will interact in the article in order to characterize the term proposed: global-personal act, where cognition is a component thereof. In the Philosophical level, we essentially lay basis on the contributions made by Aristotle and Leonardo Polo. In the psychological level we mainly look upon Erikson, with some references to Viktor Frankl and Carl Rogers. On the Neuroscience level, it is not possible to take hand of identified lines of thought, but of most recent investigations.

References

Altarejos, F., & Naval, C. (2000). Filosofía de la educación (Tercera). Pamplona: EUNSA.

Andrews-hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain’s Default Network. Neuron, 65(4), 550-562. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005.Functional-Anatomic

Aristóteles. (2014). Ética a Nicómaco. Madrid: Gredos.

Baus, C., Sebanz, N., Fuente, V. de la, Branzi, F. M., Martin, C., & Costa, A. (2014). On predicting others’ words: Electrophysiological evidence of prediction in speech production. Cognition, 133(2), 395-407. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.07.006

Behrendt, R. (2013). Conscious experience and episodic memory?: hippocampus at the crossroads. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(304). http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00304

Blakemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 2(8), 561-7. http://doi.org/10.1038/35086023

Brodski, A., Paasch, G.-F., Helbling, S., & Wibral, M. (2015). The Faces of Predictive Coding. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(24), 8997-9006. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1529-14.2015

Buckner, R. ., Andrews-Hanna, J. ., & Schacter, D. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci., 1124, 1-38. http://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.011.

Caouette, J. D., & Guyer, A. E. (2014). Gaining Insight into Adolescent Vulnerability for Social Anxiety from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 8, 65-76. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.10.003

Carey, T. A., & Mullan, R. J. (2004). What is Socratic questioning? Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 41(3), 217-226. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.apollolibrary.com/10.1037/0033-3204.41.3.217

Clark, B. C., Mahato, N., Nakazawa, M., Law, T., & Thomas, J. (2014). The Power of the Mind: The Cortex as a Critical Determinant of Muscle Strength/Weakness. Journal of Neurophysiology, 112(2), 3219-26. Recuperado a partir de http://jn.physiology.org/content/early/2014/09/24/jn.00386.2014.abstract

Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 3(3), 201-15. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755

Dulit, E. (1972). Adolescent Thinking: à la Piaget: The formal stage. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 1(4), 281-301.

Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2010). Intention-Mediated Selective Helping in Infancy. Psychological Science , 21(4), 523-527. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610364119

Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the cycle of life: selected papers. Psychological issues, 1(1), 5-165.

Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhodd and society (2nd ed.). New York: W.W: Norton.

Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: youth and crisis. New York: W.W: Norton.

Erikson, E. H. (1997). El ciclo vital completo (Española). Bracelona: Paidos.

Florian, R. V. (2007). Reinforcement Learning Through Modulation of Spike-Timing-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity. Neural Computation, 19(6), 1468-1502. http://doi.org/10.1162/neco.2007.19.6.1468

Frank, M. C., & Goodman, N. D. (2014). Inferring word meanings by assuming that speakers are informative. Cognitive Psychology, 75, 80-96. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.08.002

Frankl, V. E. (1987). El hombre doliente. Barcelona: Herder.

Frisina, W. G. (2002). The unity of knowledge and action. Towards a nonrepresentational theory of knowledge. New York: State University of New York Press.

Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: experiencing the future. Science (New York, N.Y.), 317(5843), 1351-4. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1144161

Gill, C. (1985). Plato and the Education of Character. Archiv fur Geschichte der Philosophie, 67(1), 1-26. http://doi.org/10.1515/agph.1985.67.1.1

Glöckner, A., Hilbig, B. E., & Jekel, M. (2014). What is adaptive about adaptive decision making? A parallel constraint satisfaction account. Cognition, 133(3), 641-666. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.017

Hamann, K., Warneken, F., Greenberg, J. R., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees. Nature, 476(7360), 328-331. Recuperado a partir de http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature10278

Hamlin, J. K. (2013). Moral Judgment and Action in Preverbal Infants and Toddlers: Evidence for an Innate Moral Core . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 22(3), 186-193. http://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412470687

He, K., Huertas, M., Hong, S. Z., Tie, X., Hell, J. W., Shouval, H., & Kirkwood, A. (2015). Distinct Eligibility Traces for LTP and LTD in Cortical Synapses. Neuron, 88(3), 528–538. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.037

Herwig, A., & Schneider, W. X. (2014). Predicting object features across saccades: Evidence from object recognition and visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 143(5), 1903-1922.

Herzog, M. H., & Clarke, A. M. (2014). Why vision is not both hierarchical and feedforward. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 8(135). http://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2014.00135

Heuvel, M. P. Van Den, & Sporns, O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(12), 683-696. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.012

Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679-709. http://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.109.4.679

Huertas, M. A., Schwettmann, S., Kirkwood, A., & Shouval, H. (2014). Stable reinforcement learning via temporal competition between LTP and LTD traces. BMC Neuroscience, 15(Suppl 1), O12-O12. http://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-15-S1-O12

Ibos, G., & Freedman, D. J. (2015). Dynamic Integration of Task-Relevant Visual Features in Posterior Parietal Cortex. Neuron, 83(6), 1468-1480. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.020

Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.

Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees Are Rational Maximizers in an Ultimatum Game. Science , 318(5847), 107-109. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1145850

Juravle, G. (2015). Compression and suppression as instances of a similar mechanism affecting tactile perception during movement. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(217). Recuperado a partir de http://www.frontiersin.org/Journal/FullText.aspx?s=537&name=human_neuroscience&ART_DOI=10.3389/fnhum.2015.00217

Kaplan, R. (2014). Endogenous fMRI default mode network fluctuations both positively and negatively correlate with individual transfer of learning. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8(229). http://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00229

Kohlberg, L., & Hersh, R. H. (1977). Moral Development: A Review of the Theory. Theory Into Practice, 16(2), 53-59. Recuperado a partir de http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=pbh&AN=5201785&lang=es&site=ehost-live

Kohlberg, L., & Mayer, R. (1972). Development as the aim of education. Harvard Education Review, 4(4), 449-496.

Kolb, D. A. (2015). Experiential Learning. Experience as the source of learning development (Second). New Yersey: Pearson Education.

Kurumada, C., Brown, M., Bibyk, S., Pontillo, D. F., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2014). Is it or isn’t it: Listeners make rapid use of prosody to infer speaker meanings. Cognition, 133(2), 335-342. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.017

Leijenhorst, L. Van, Zanolie, K., Van, C. S., Westenberg, P. M., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Crone, E. A., … Crone, E. A. (2010). What Motivates the Adolescent? Brain Regions Mediating Reward Sensitivity across Adolescence. Cerebral cortex, 20(1), 61-69. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp078

Ling, S., Pratte, M. S., & Tong, F. (2015). Attention alters orientation processing in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nat Neurosci, 18, 496–498. Recuperado a partir de http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3967

Lipman, M. (2016). El lugar del pensamiento en la educación. Barcelona: Octaedro.

Mantini, D., & Vanduffel, W. (2013). Emerging roles of the brain’s default network. The Neuroscientist?: a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry, 19(1), 76-87. http://doi.org/10.1177/1073858412446202

McAuliffe, K., Blake, P. R., Kim, G., Wrangham, R. W., & Warneken, F. (2013). Social Influences on Inequity Aversion in Children. PLoS ONE, 8(12), e80966. Recuperado a partir de http://dx.doi.org/10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0080966

Milinski, M. (2013). Chimps play fair in the ultimatum game. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 110(6), 1978-1979. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1222132110

Miloyan, B., & Suddendorf, T. (2015). Feelings of the future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(4), 196–200. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.008

Morita, K., & Kato, A. (2014). Striatal dopamine ramping may indicate flexible reinforcement learning with forgetting in the cortico-basal ganglia circuits. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 8(36). http://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2014.00036

Murillo, J. I. (1999). ¿Por qué es tan difícil vivir una vida? Lo uno y lo múltiple en las tendencias humanas. En J. Aranguren (Ed.), La libertad sentimental (pp. 29-52). Pamplona: Cuadernos de anuario filosófico.

Narvaez, D. (2008). Triune ethics: The neurobiological roots of our multiple moralities. New Ideas in Psychology, 26(1), 95-119. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.07.008

Nelson, H. J., Kendall, G. E., & Shields, L. (2014). Neurological and Biological Foundations of Children’s Social and Emotional Development: An Integrated Literature Review. The Journal of School Nursing, 30(4), 240-250. http://doi.org/10.1177/1059840513513157

Oosterwijk, S., Lindquist, K., Anderson, E., Dautoff, R., Moriguchi, Y., & Barrett, L. (2012). States of mind: emotions, body feelings, and thoughts share distributed neural networks. NeuroImage, 62(3), 210-28. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.079

Orón Semper, J. V. (2014). Neurociencia y fe?: El sistema de creencias como lugar de encuentro interdisciplinar ( Neuroscience and Faith?: The belief system as a venue of interdisciplinary meeting ). Scientia et fides, 2(2), 213-270.

Pardo-vázquez, J. L., & Acuña, C. (2014). Bases neurales de las decisiones perceptivas?: papel de la corteza premotora ventral. Rev Neurol, 58(9), 401-410.

Perlovsky, L. (2013). A challenge to human evolution — cognitive dissonance. Frontiers in psychology, 4(179), 1-3. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00179

Peters, B., Kaiser, J., Rahm, B., & Bledowski, C. (2015). Activity in Human Visual and Parietal Cortex Reveals Object-Based Attention in Working Memory. The Journal of Neuroscience , 35(8), 3360-3369. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3795-14.2015

Peters, R. S. (1966). Ethics and education. New York: Routledge.

Pitskel, N. B., Bolling, D. Z., Kaiser, M. D., Crowley, M. J., & Pelphrey, K. a. (2011). How grossed out are you? The neural bases of emotion regulation from childhood to adolescence. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 1(3), 324-37. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.03.004

Polo, L. (1998). Antropologia transcendental. Tomo I. La persona humana. Pamplona: EUNSA.

Polo, L. (2007). Persona y libertad. Pamplona: EUNSA.

Proctor, D., Williamson, R. A., de Waal, F. B. M., & Brosnan, S. F. (2013). Chimpanzees play the ultimatum game. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 110(6), 2070-2075. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1220806110

Rangel, A., Camerer, C., & Montague, P. R. (2008). A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 9(7), 545-56. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2357

Rogers, C. R. (2000). El proceso de convertirse en persona, mi técnica terapéutica. Barcelona: Paidos Iberica.

Sellés, J. F. (2015). ¿es trascendental la antropología de viktor Frankl? Madrid: Ápeiron Ediciones.

Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., & Premack, D. (2012). Do Infants Have a Sense of Fairness? Psychological Science , 23(2), 196-204. Recuperado a partir de http://pss.sagepub.com/content/23/2/196.abstract

Stalnaker, T. A., Cooch, N. K., & Schoenbaum, G. (2015). What the orbitofrontal cortex does not do. Nat Neurosci, 18(5), 620-627. Recuperado a partir de http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3982

Steinmann, E., Schmalor, A., Prehn-Kristensen, A., Wolff, S., Galka, A., Möhring, J., … Siniatchkin, M. (2014). Developmental changes of neuronal networks associated with strategic social decision-making. Neuropsychologia, 56, 37-46. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.025

Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

van den Heuvel, M. P., & Sporns, O. (2011). Rich-club organization of the human connectome. The Journal of neuroscience?: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(44), 15775-86. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011

Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Parental Presence and Encouragement Do Not Influence Helping in Young Children. Infancy, 18(3), 345-368. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00120.x

Published

2018-01-04

Issue

Section

Articles